Human language
is made possible by special adaptations of the human mind and body that
occurred in the course of human evolution, and which are put to use by children
in acquiring their mother tongue.
- Evolution of Language
The scientific question is
whether the chimps' abilities are homologous to human language that is, whether
the two systems show the same basic organization owing to descent from a single
system in their common ancestor. For example, biologists don't debate whether the
wing-like structures of gliding rodents may be called "genuine wings"
or something else (a boring question of definitions). It's clear that these
structures are not homologous to the wings of bats, because they have a
fundamentally different anatomical plan, reflecting a different evolutionary
history. Bats' wings are modifications of the hands of the common mammalian
ancestor; flying squirrels' wings are modifications of its rib cage. The two
structures are merely analogous: similar in function.
This lack of homology does
not, by the way, cast doubt on a gradualistic Darwinian account of language
evolution. Humans did not evolve directly from chimpanzees. Both derived from
common ancestor, probably around 6-7 million years ago. This leaves about 300,000
generations in which language could have evolved gradually in the lineage
leading to humans, after it split off from the lineage leading to chimpanzees.
Presumably language evolved in the human lineage for two reasons: our ancestors
developed technology and knowledge of the local environment in their lifetimes,
and were involved in extensive reciprocal cooperation. This allowed them to
benefit by sharing hard-won knowledge with their kin and exchanging it with
their neighbors (Pinker & Bloom, 1990).
- Dissociations between Language and General Intelligence
Humans
evolved brain circuitry, mostly in the left hemisphere surrounding the sylvian
fissure, that appears to be designed for language, though how exactly their
internal wiring gives rise to rules of language is unknown (see the Chapter by
Zurif). The brain mechanisms underlying language are not just those allowing us
to be smart in general. Strokes often leave adults with catastrophic losses in
language (see the Chapter by Zurif, and Pinker, 1994a). By definition,
Specifically Language Impaired people show such deficits despite the absence of
cognitive problems like retardation, sensory problems like hearing loss, or
social problems like autism.
More interestingly, there are
syndromes showing the opposite dissociation, where intact language coexists
with severe retardation. These cases show that language development does not
depend on fully functioning general intelligence. One example comes from
children with Spina Bifida, a malformation of the vertebrae that leaves the
spinal cord unprotected, often resulting in hydrocephalus, an increase in
pressure in the cerebrospinal fluid filling the ventricles (large cavities) of
the brain, distending the brain from within. Hydrocephalic children occasionally
end up significantly retarded but can carry on long, articulate, and fully
grammatical conversations, in which they earnestly recount vivid events that
are, in fact, products of their imaginations (Cromer, 1992; Curtiss, 1989;
Pinker, 1994a). Another example is Williams Syndrome, an inherited condition
involving physical abnormalities, significant retardation (the average IQ is
about 50), incompetence at simple everyday tasks (tying shoelaces, finding
one's way, adding two numbers, and retrieving items from a cupboard), social
warmth and gregariousness, and fluent, articulate language abilities (Bellugi,
et al., 1990).
- Maturation of the Language SystemAs the chapter by Newport and Gleitman suggests, the maturation of language circuits during a child's early years may be a driving force underlying the course of language acquisition (Pinker, 1994, Chapter 9; Bates, Thal, & Janowsky, 1992; Locke, 1992; Huttenlocher, 1990). Before birth, virtually all the neurons (nerve cells) are formed, and they migrate into their proper locations in the brain. But head size, brain weight, and thickness of the cerebral cortex (gray matter), where the synapses (junctions) subserving mental computation take place, continue to increase rapidly in the year after birth. Long-distance connections (white matter) are not complete until nine months, and they continue to grow their speed-inducing myelin insulation throughout childhood. Synapses continue to develop, peaking in number between nine months and two years (depending on the brain region), at which point the child has 50% more synapses than the adult. Many explanations have been advanced for children's superiority: they can exploit the special ways that their mothers talk them, they make errors unself-consciously, they are more motivated to communicate, they like to conform, they are not xenophobic or set in their ways, and they have no first language to interfere.
1 komentar:
I still liked this article, good, good content, and unique design.Thank you for sharing the article.
aladinqq
Post a Comment